According
to Wikipedia, retirement is the withdrawal from one’s position or occupation or
from one’s active working life. Retirement, or the practice of leaving one’s job
or ceasing to work after reaching a certain age, has been around since the 18th
century. Prior to the 18th century, humans had an average life expectancy
between 26 and 40 years. In consequence, only a small percentage of the
population reached an age where physical impairments began to be obstacles to
working. As life expectancy increases and more and more people live to an advanced
age. Ash (1) examined retirement by designating it as a reward for a lifetime
of work.
-
Continuity
or crisis?
They
spend in leisure roles as age increases (Riley & Foner, 1968). Upon
retirement, leisure pursuits occupy a great deal of the individual’s time, and
there is a question as to whether leisure roles can fill the void left by work.
There is little doubt that leisure can fill the time formerly occupied by work,
but the problem is whether leisure is capable of giving the individual the kind
of self-respect and identity that he got from the job.
· People got the kind of self-respect and identity from the job.
The identity crisis theory
Miller
(1965) took the following position:
1.
Retirement
is basically degrading because
although there is an implication that retirement is a right that is earned
though life-long labor, there is also a tacit understanding that this reward is
being given primarily to coax the individual from a role he is no longer able
to play.
2.
Occupational identity
invades all of the other areas of the person’s life. Accordingly, the
father and head of household roles, the friend role, and even leisure roles are
mediated by the individual’s occupational identity.
3.
The
identity that comes from work is related to deeply ingrained values as to which roles can give a legitimate identity.
4.
Leisure
roles cannot replace work as a source of self-respect and identity because it
is not supported by norms that would make this legitimate. That is, the retired
person does not feel justified in deriving self-respect from leisure. Leisure
is simple not defined as a legitimate source of self-respect by the general
population.
5.
Beyond
the simple need to be doing something there is a need to be engaged in
something that is defined by most people as utilitarian or gainful in some way.
Thus, the stamp collector must emphasize the financial rewards, paintings are
offered for sale, or woodworking is confined to immediately “useful”
objectives. In short, the only kinds of
leisure that can provide identity are work-substitutes.
· People got a sense
of self-respect and identity from work.
6.
There
is a stigma of “implied inability to
perform” that is associated with retirement and carried over into all of the
individual’s remaining roles and that results in an identity breakdown.
7.
Identity
breakdown involves a process whereby the
individual’s former claims to prestige or status are invalidated by the implied
inability to perform, and this proves embarrassing for the stigmatized person.
Miller calls this result “the portent of embarrassment.”
8.
Embarrassment
leads to the individual’s withdrawal from the situation or prevents him from
participating to begin with.
9.
The
answer lies not in inventing new roles
for the aging, but rather in “determining
what roles presently exist in the social system … offering vicarious satisfactions,
that can reduce the socially debilitating
loss accompanying occupational retirement.”
10. Miller implies that creating an ethic which would make full-time leisure an acceptable activity for a worthwhile person is a possible way to resolve the dilemma of the retired leisure participant.
Miller’s analysis of the situation is an insightful one. Nevertheless, it rests on the assumption that prior to retirement the individual derived his identity primarily from his jobs. Also implied in Miller’s identity crisis theory is the assumption that most people want to stay on the job, since this is their main identity, and that therefore most retirement is involuntary. This is no doubt related to the fact Miller leaves out of his discussion those who retired voluntarily. Miller also implies that he subscribes to the activity theory of adjustment to aging since he assumes that los roles need to be replaced (Havinghurst, 1963).
Evidence concerning identity crisis
There are several sets of questions which thus emerge from an examination of the identity crisis theory presented by Miller: is his portrayal of the relationship between involuntary retirement and leisure an accurate one?, is the pattern, even if accurate, typical of most older leisure participants?, and what is the pattern among those who are voluntarily retired?
1.
Retirement
has been found to result in a loss of a sense of involvement, but this was
unrelated to other self-concept variables of optimism and autonomy (Back &
Guptill, 1966).
Disengagement theory tells us to expect some withdrawal from involvement, and it is noteworthy that this loss of involvement does not appear to have adverse results for other aspects of the self-concept. This leads to skepticism concerning Miller’s “portent of embarrassment.”
2.
Strong
work-orientation is frequently found among retired people, but this is not
accompanied by anxiety, depression, dislike of retirement, or withdrawal from
activity (Cottrell & Atchley, 1969).
Our findings indicate that a strong
positive orientation toward work “exists apart from the job itself but … has no
import for the individual apart from the job itself.” In terms of adjustment,
there was apparently no negative result from carrying a positive orientation
toward work into retirement.
3.
When
men retired from upper-white-collar, middle status, and semi-skilled jobs were
compared, it was found that the upper-white-collar people had internalized occupationally-oriented
norms. Middle-status workers were oriented toward specific tasks and situations
often resulting in the acquisition of skills that were transferable to leisure
situations. Semi-skilled workers were engaged mainly in activities oriented
about things (Simpson, Back & McKinney, 1966).
Of these occupational strata, the upper-white-collar
stratum comes closest to Miller’s model of the retired person. These are
work-oriented people. However, neither of the other two strata fit the work-oriented
model. Middle-status people develop skills on the job that carry over into
other roles. Thus, the salesman may carry his smooth-talking style over into
his leisure roles. Semi-skilled people are oriented around the job, but not
necessarily because they have any deep abiding commitment to the job. For them
it may be purely a matter of not having been trained for anything other than a
job.
4.
(…)
5.
In
addition, data from retired railroaders indicate that there are continuities in
the situations people face that minimize
the impact of retirement (Cottrell, 1970). Family, friends, church, and
other roles continue despite retirement. Cottrell’s data suggest that the
portent of embarrassment and loss of identity is minimized by the tendency to
select friends on the job from among those of one’s own age. The end results of
this process is to create retirement cohorts of people who have known each
other on the job and who retire together. (…) It results in a group of retired
friends who have known each other for years and whose concepts of each other involved
a great deal more than the mere playing of an occupational role. Nevertheless,
this group is also capable of sustaining the prestige gained on the job because
they know all about how this prestige was generated.
To the extent that older people are geographically
mobile, they might tend to lose these continuities, but most retired people,
particularly the semi- or unskilled, do not move away from their place of
long-term residence (Riley & Foner, 1968).
6.
Cottrell’s
data (1970) also indicate that as the
concept of retirement is incorporated into the culture, the tendency to look
upon work as a temporary part of life increases.
The implication here is that if work is
not a permanent part of life, then one puts greater emphasis on other parts of
life that are more permanent. For example, if a man knows the day he begins
working that he will work 25 years and then quit, he is very likely to avoid
letting work become an all-consuming part of his life.
7.
(…)
8.
Nearly
two-thirds of retired men retired as a result of their own decision. Less than
1 in 5 was retired involuntarily as a result of their own decision. Less than 1
in 5 was retired in voluntarily as a result of reaching retirement age
(Cottrell & Atchley, 1969; Riley & Foner, 1968).
By leaving out those in poor health and those who voluntarily retired, Miller (1965) effectively limited the group he was talking about to less than a third of the retired men and an even smaller proportion of the retired women.
Identity
continuity theory
It
may seem that we have dwelt too long and too deeply with the relation between leisure
and retirement. Nevertheless, if we are to understand the nature of leisure
among older people, it must be put in its proper context. Miller’s position is
a very common one and is constantly being used as a basis for decisions that
influence older people’s lives. Our detailed examination of this approach has
shown it to be at least questionable and very possibly false.
To
begin with, there is evidence in the Scripps Foundation studies and elsewhere
that the adjustment problems sometimes associated with retirement are not the
result of the loss of work and the identity it provides. In fact, a highly
positive orientation toward work had little influence on retirement adjustment.
There is no indication that highly work-oriented people are unable to take up
leisure roles; in fact, just the opposite. We
could find no concrete evidence that retirement in and of itself negatively
influences the quality of one’s family life, friendships, or associations.
Accordingly,
an alternative to Miller’s identity crisis theory of the relationship between
retirement and leisure might contain the following points:
Many people are never highly work-oriented and thus they may very well provide a model for others concerning what it would be like to derive self-satisfaction from leisure. In addition, the ethic of the system allows this as long as the money used to lead a life of leisure is legitimately earned.
Self-respect can be gained from leisure pursuits in retirement if (a) the individual has enough money, and (b) he has a cohort of retired friends who will accept his full-time leisure as legitimate and help him to negate the stigma of implied inability to perform, if such a stigma exists.
As retirement becomes more and more an expected part of the life-cycle, this orientation should spread beyond the cohort of friends. In any event, the retired individual will continue to see himself as a railroader, teacher, etc. even though he longer plays the role. Thus, the crux of this alternative theory is identity continuity.
(…)
The identity continuity theory and the data which give rise to it suggest that
leisure can have a great deal of positive value as a bridge between pre- and
post- retirement life and that this value will increase in the future.
On
Miller’s identity crisis model & continuity model
-
Adjustment to loss of job at retirement
How
do people adjust to retirement? (…) Apparently most people respond favorably to
life in retirement. In our study of retired teachers and telephone company
employees, not quite 30 percent felt that they would never get used to
retirement. Harris found that 33 percent of his sample viewed retirement as
less than satisfactory. Streib and Schneider found that about 10 percent of
their sample rejected retirement by
taking another job, and these people tended to be high on a scale of job
deprivation, a measure of the extent to which they missed their jobs.
(…)
Retirement per se is directly responsible for only two of these sources of
difficulty: missing one’s job and financial problems. Health problems are
almost always a cause rather than a consequence of retirement. (…)
Theory is a necessary element in the development of knowledge. Theory serves to identify gaps in knowledge and as a guide for organizing research. With regard to adjustment to retirement, theories are needed to describe and explain 1) adjustment to changes in income and 2) adjustment to no longer having job responsibilities.
As yet no theory concerning how people adjust to income decline has been developed. What is needed is descriptive data on the phases of adjustment and on the strategies that are employed. A decision model would probably be useful. However, at this point the basic descriptive data necessary to begin to develop such a theory have not been assembled.
More attention has been given to the problem of adjusting to the loss of one’s job. Several theories of adjustment have emerged, each of which has a different emphasis.
1)
Activity
theory
Activity theory assumes that the job means different things to different people and that to adjust successfully to the loss of one’s job, one must find a substitute for whatever personal goal the job was used to achieve. The most often quoted proponents of this theory are Friedmann and Havighurst and Miller. Friedmann and Havighurst approach the matter in terms of substitute activities, and Miller carried it one step further to include substitutes activities, and Miller carried it one step further to include substitute activities which serve as new sources of identity. The assumption here is that the individual will seek and find a work substitute. In a test of this theory, however, Shanas found it to be of very limited utility when applied to American society. In my own research, activity theory has fit the behavior of only a tiny proportion of retired people
2)
Continuity
theory
Continuity theory assumes that, whenever possible, the individual will cope with retirement by increasing the time spent in roles he already plays rather than by finding new roles to play. This assumption is based on the finding that older people tend to stick with tried and true ways rather than to experiment, and on the assumption that most retired people want their life in retirement to be as much like their preretirement life as possible. However, continuity theory allows for a gradual reduction in overall activity. Obviously, these basic assumptions do not fit all retired people, although they may fit the majority.
3)
Disengagement
theory
Disengagement theory holds that retirement is a necessary manifestation of the mutual withdrawal of society and the older individual from one another as a consequence of the increased prospect of biological failure in the individual organism. This theory has been criticized for making the rejection of older people by society seem “natural” and, therefore, right. However, the fact of the matter is that many people do want to withdraw from full-time jobs and welcome the opportunity to do so. Streib and Schneider refined disengagement theory to apply more directly to the realities of retirement.
4)
Differential
disengagement
Differential disengagement is the term they use to reflect the idea that disengagement can occur at different rates for different roles. And they do not contend that disengagement is irreversible. They hold that by removing the necessity for energy-sapping labor on a job, retirement may free the individual with declining energy to increase his level of engagement in other spheres of life. Such increases in involvement do in fact occur.
(…)
Hazardous
as it may be, I would like to offer another approach to understanding adjustment
to the loss of job which accompanies retirement. (…) In sum, the central
processes of adjustment are held to be internal
compromise and interpersonal negotiations. While these two elements
interact quite strongly in real situations, for analytical purposes I shall
treat them separately.
TO BE CONTINUED.
Comments
Post a Comment